Ad Code

A Phonological Study of Hausa Short Genitive Linker: -r/-n

Cite this article as: Adamu U. A., Saleh A. L. & Baraya I (2025). A Phonological Study of Hausa Short Genitive Linker: -r/-n Zamfara International Journal of Humanities, 3(3), 1-4. www.doi.org/10.36349/zamijoh.2025.v03i03.001.

A PHONOLOGICAL STUDY OF HAUSA SHORT GENITIVE LINKER: -R/-N

Usman Ali Adamu
Department of Languages and Linguistics
University of Maiduguri

Ajid Lawan Saleh PhD
Department of Languages and Linguistics
University of Maiduguri

Ibrahim Baraya
Department of Hausa Language
Adamawa State College of Education, Hong

Abstract: This study is on the phonological study of Hausa short genitive linker –r and -n. Descriptive method is used in analyzing the data extracted from the existing literature (Galadanci 1976, Newman 2000, Abubakar 2001, and Jaggar 2001). This study found that, from phonological point of view, the Hausa short genitive linker (-r/-n) in rapid speech, there is only partial assimilation of the short genitive linker “-n” with a following obstruent across word boundary in term of place of articulation but not in term of manner of articulation. The only position where assimilation within word boundary occur is when the enclitic “-r” is followed by a short possessive pronoun within same word boundary. However, there is also a constraint on the assimilation process across word boundary when the following onset of the first syllable across word boundary is non liquid sound. Also it is found that, the tone of the short genitive is not the same as that of the preceding syllable as earlier claim by Galadanci (1976) nor toneless as claim by Jaggar (2001), rather, the tone of these short genitive –n/-r supersedes the low tone of the last syllable of the head NP; also, when the tone of the last syllable of the head NP is high, the high tone and the floating low tone coalesced into fallen tone. Moreover, the short genitive linker, when used as referential, it functions as neutralization marker. Because it conditioned the neutralization of the mid front unrounded vowel /ee/ and mid back rounded vowel /oo/ to short low central unrounded [a]. Finally, it is found that, when the short genitive linker –n is used as possessive marker, it conditioned the deletion of the glided vowel ([i] of /ai/ and /u/ of /au/) of the diphthongs. This process of vowel change support the claim made by Abubakar (1983) that in Hausa, the diphthong /ai/ and /au/ are combination of vowel /a/ plus approximate vowels /j/ or /w/. This occur as a result of the deletion of the approximate vowels because sequence of three consonants at the word final position is not permissible in the language in question.

1.0 INTRODUCTION

Genitive linker is one of the grammatical elements that is usually used for connecting various grammatical words. In Hausa, the short genitive linker is either “-n”, which marks for masculine singular as well as plural; while “-r”, marks for feminine singular only (see Galadanci 1976, Newman 2000, Abubakar 2001, Jaggar 2001 and Crysman 2009). However, this paper focus on Hausa short genitive linker (-r/-n) from phonological point of view and a descriptive method is used in analyzing the data extracted from the existing literature (Galadanci 1976, Newman 2000, Abubakar 2001, and Jaggar 2001).

2.0 LITERATURE REVIEW

Phonologically, Galadanci (1976), while discussing genitive linker, states that the tone of Hausa genitive is same as that of the preceding syllable. Consider:

-n baya - ciwòǹ baya

-r wata - tsaiwáŕ wata

-n Bala - dawakíń Bala

In the above examples, it is shown that the genitive linker assimilates automatically to the tone of the coda of its lexical head. This assimilation occurs when the following word is a lexical noun. On the contrary, Newman (2000), morphophonologically, sees short genitive in Hausa when used as referential as an element consisting of a floating low tone followed by a consonant “-n/-r”. The low tone conditionally surfaces as fallen tone if preceded by a syllable with high tone depending on the gender and number of the word it is attached to. Consider:

Yaròn  -  the boy (m.) previously referred to.

Tunkìyâr -  the sheep (f.) previously referred to.

Dabbobîn -  the animals (pl.) previously referred to.

In addition, the linker “-n” assimilates automatically to the position of the following consonant within the same word or across word boundary. Consider:

Dokinka [dookìŋka] “your horse”

Dokin kanar [dokiŋkanar]

Furthermore, when the enclitic “-n” is followed by a bilabial nasal, it produces a surface geminate. Consider:

Gidanmu [gidammu] “our house”

The assimilation is total when “-n” is followed by liquids which depends on speech tempo (Newman 2000). Consider:

Kawun Ladi [kaawùllaadì]

However, the enclitic “-r” on the other hand assimilates to the following abutting consonant within same word boundary. Consider

‘yarsa [ ‘yassa].

Thus, assimilation is obligatory with the demonstrative as identify by Newman (2000). Consider:

Bishiyar nan → bishiyan nan

Corroboratively, Jaggar (2001) explains that the attachment of the enclitic linker conditioned the automatic shortening of long vowel of the last closed syllable of a word. Consider:

Mootàr malami ce  “it’s the teacher’s car”.

In the above example, the long vowel /aa/ of the last syllable of the noun “mootaa” has been shorten due to the presence of the enclitic “-r”.

Additionally, the linker assimilates with abutting consonant in some instances (as also mentioned by Newman 2000). Consider:

Motar mamman [ motam mammaŋ]

Mostly, a short high front vowel /i/ is epenthesis before the enclitic –n linker in certain nouns that are loan from Arabic and English that have closed final syllable (Jaggar ibid). Consider:

Ladanin garinmu – the muezzin of our town

Furthermore, Crysman (2009) explains that, phonologically, when a strong verbal noun precedes an object pronoun, it takes the low tone genitive set (-sà) not the accusative set that operates with the weak verbal nouns or main verb. Consider:

yaanaa tàmbayàrsà “ he is/was asking him”.

However, the reduction of the non-pronominal possessors “na/ta” (free genitive linker) to “-n/-r” phonologically deals with the deletion of nucleus of the monosyllabic non-pronominal possessors, as well as resyllabification of the last syllable of the preceding word by making it the coda of the syllable with automatic shortening of the vowel of the last syllable. In addition, the rule of rhotatism is what is applicable in the case of “ta” → ”-r”(Crysman 2009). consider:

maataa ta Bello “Bello’s mother”(sic)

maatar Bello   “Bello’s mother”(sic).

In this section, we have reviewed related literature on the Hausa short genitive linker (-r/-n) from phonological point of view. In the following section, we will discuss the data collected and contribution will be made.

3.0 DISCUSSION

In Hausa, the short genitive linker (-r/-n) is phonologically realize contextually as discussed by various scholars. In rapid speech, there is only partial assimilation of the short genitive linker “-n” with a following obstruent across word boundary in term of place of articulation but not in term of manner of articulation.

Consider:

Kawun Garba [kaawùŋgarba]

*Kawug Garba            [kaawùggarba]

Kawun Kande             [kaawùŋkande]

*Kawuk Kade             [kaawùkkande]

ramin cinnaka             [raminʧinnaka]

*ramic cinnaka            [ramiʧʧinnaka]

Dokin Bala                  [dakimbaalaa]

*Dokin Bala    [dakibbaalaa]

The only position where assimilation within same word boundary occur is when the enclitic “-r” is followed by a short possessive pronoun within same word boundary. Consider:

‘yarka              [‘yakka]  “your daughter”

‘yarmu             [‘yammu] “our daughter”

‘yarta               [‘yatta]   “her daughter”

‘yarsa               [‘yassa]   “his daughter”

The above examples show that the linker “-r” assimilates automatically to the position of the following abutting consonant (short possessive pronoun) within same word boundary. However, there is constrain on this process across word boundary when the following onset of the first syllable across word boundary is non liquid sound. Consider:

‘yar Asabe       [‘yarɁasabe]  “Asabe’s daughter/ a girl born on Saturday”

*‘yar Asabe     [‘yaɁɁasabe]  “Asabe’s daughter/ a girl born on Saturday”

motar Garba    [mootargarba]  “Garba’s car”

*motar Garba [mootaggarba]  “Garba’s car”

‘yar Bala         “Bala’s daughter”

* ‘yab Bala      *[ ‘yabbala] “Bala’s daughter”

Mootar firsononi         “prisoners’ van”

*Mootaf firsononi       *[mootaffirsononi] “prisoners’ van”

maatar Bello     “Bello’s wife”

* maatab Bello *[maatabbello] “Bello’s wife”.

In the above examples, the linker “-r” doesn’t assimilate to the position of the following consonant across word boundary. Nevertheless, this process across word boundary is possible when the following onset of the first syllable across word boundary is liquid sound. Consider:

faɽar-wutaa [faɽawwutaa] “Sulphur”

As observed, the tone of the short genitive is not the same as that of the preceding syllable as earlier claim by Galadanci (1976) nor toneless as claim by Jaggar (2001), rather, short genitive –n/-r has tone. Firstly, the tone of this short genitive –n/-r supersedes the low tone of the last syllable of the head NP; secondly, when the tone of the last syllable of the head NP is high, the high tone and the floating low tone coalesced into fallen tone. Consider:

 tunkìya -  sheep (f.)

 tunkìyâr -  the sheep (f.) previously referred to.

In the above examples, the first example has high tone on its last syllable and has no genitive linker attached to it. As for the second example, the high tone of the last syllable and the floating low tone of the short genitive linker coalesced into fallen tone of the last syllable.

Moreover, One of the function of the short genitive linker –n/-r is that it is used as referential marker. Morphophonologically, when the short genitive linker, is used as referential, it functions as neutralization marker. Because it conditioned the neutralization of the mid front unrounded vowel /ee/ and mid back rounded vowel /oo/ to short low central unrounded [a]. consider:

Karee +n → /karaŋ/   “the dog”

Zoomoo +n → /zoomaŋ/ “the hare”

In the above examples, the mid front unrounded vowel /ee/ and mid back rounded vowel /oo/ are neutralized to short low central unrounded [a] because of the short genitive linker –n which serves as referential marker. It is also clear that the short genitive linker –n is also realized as velar nasal/ŋ/ at word final position. In addition, this process is also the same when the short genitive linker –n is used as possessive marker. Consider:

Keken Bala /kj eekj aŋ balaa/  Bala’s bicycle

In Hausa phonology, there are various instances where deletion occur. When the short genitive linker –n is used as possessive marker, it conditioned the deletion of the glided vowel ([i] of /ai/ and /u/ of /au/) of the Hausa diphthongs. Consider:

 /rai/                 “soul”

 /ransa/            “lit. his soul” (his life)

/mai/                “oil”

/manta/                        “her oil”

/kai/                 “head”

/kansa/             “his head”

/tabarau/          “spectacle”

/tabaransa/       “his spectacle”

In the above examples, it is observed that the diphthongs /ai/ and /au/ become [a]. This process of vowel change support the claim made by Abubakar (1983) that in Hausa, the diphthong /ai/ and /au/ are combination of vowel /a/ plus approximate vowels /j/ or /w/. This occur as a result of the deletion of the approximate vowels because sequence of three consonants word final is not possible. Consider:

Mai  “oil”

Maajii < maaj-ii </ mai / “oil”

mai +nta →/manta/ “her oil”

*maj +nta →/majnta/

In the above examples, the sequence of vowel /a/ plus approximate /j/ making the diphthong /ai/ and short genitive linker –n as well as the short possessive feminine marker “ta” (/-ajnt-/ conditioned the deletion of the approximate /j/.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, it is seen that from the phonological point of view, the Hausa short genitive linker (-r/-n) in rapid speech, there is only partial assimilation of the short genitive linker “-n” with a following obstruent across word boundary in term of place of articulation but not in term of manner of articulation. The only position where assimilation within same word boundary occur is when the enclitic “-r” is followed by a short possessive pronoun within same word boundary. However, there is also a constrain on assimilation process across word boundary when the following unset of the first syllable across word boundary is non liquid sound. As observed, the tone of the short genitive is not the same as that of the preceding syllable as earlier claim by Galadanci (1976) nor toneless as claim by Jaggar (2001), rather, short genitive –n/-r has tone. Firstly, the tone of this short genitive –n/-r supersedes the low tone of the last syllable of the head NP; secondly, when the tone of the last syllable of the head NP is high, the high tone and the floating low tone coalesced into fallen tone. Moreover, as seen above, the short genitive linker, when used as referential, it functions as neutralization marker. Because it conditioned the neutralization of the mid front unrounded vowel /ee/ and mid back rounded vowel /oo/ to short low central unrounded [a]. It is also found out that when the short genitive linker –n is used as possessive marker, it conditioned the deletion of the glided vowel ([i] of/ai/ and /u/ of /au/) of the Hausa diphthongs. This process of vowel change support the claim made by Abubakar (1983) that in Hausa, the diphthong /ai/ and /au/ are combination of vowel /a/ plus approximate vowels /j/ or /w/. This occur as a result of the deletion of the approximate vowels because sequence of three consonants word final is not possible.

REFERENCE

1.      Abubakar, A. (1983). Generative Phonology and Dialect Variation: A Study of Hausa Dialects. Ph.D. Thesis. Unpublished. SOAS. University of London.                                                                                                               

2.      Crysmann, B. (2005), A unified account of Hausa genitive constructions Universit¨at Bonn & Universit¨at des Saarlandes Poppelsdorfer Allee 47, D{53115 Bonn..

3.      Jaggar, P. (2001), Hausa. John Benjamins, Amsterdam.

4.      Galadanci, M. K. M. (1976) An Introduction to Hausa Grammar. Ikeja: Longman Nigeria Ltd.

5.       Newman, P. (2000), The Hausa Language. An Encyclopedic Reference Grammar,
Yale University Press, New Haven, CT
.

A Phonological Study of Hausa Short Genitive Linker: -r/-n

Post a Comment

0 Comments